Thinking about how language works

The purpose of this resource is to provide teachers with additional information about language that will help them to analyse student responses to ARB items. It will be of use when analysing responses from students with English as a first or subsequent language.

Part one: Sentence structure

  1. The nature of the information in a sentence
  2. The focus of information in a sentence
  3. The way language condenses and generalises information

Part two: Connecting and tracking ideas in text

  1. The words used to link ideas within a sentence
  2. The words used to link ideas within and between sentences

     

 

 

Part one: Sentence structure 

1. The nature of the information in a sentence

(a) Types of sentences
A sentence can be an independent clause, or can be formed by combining clauses, usually with the addition of a linking word or phrase. A clause is a grammatical structure in which several components of meaning are brought together to form a message. The verb is the essential element of any clause.

Simple sentences contain a single clause:

  • We knew each other well.
  • Have you got the time?
  • They drove to Hastings.
  • I spoke to her mother.

However, many sentences have more than one clause, so are not simple sentences:

  • Sam found his teddy bear
  • and climbed back into bed
  •  where he pulled the blankets up around his ears,
  • closed his heavy eyes,
  • and fell asleep.

Clauses can be combined in different ways to make different kinds of sentences: compound, complex, and compound-complex.

Compound Sentences
Compound sentences consist of two or more independent clauses i.e., each is capable of standing on its own and conveying a message. Each has equal status and provides equally important information. These clauses may be linked with connective words or phrases such as: "and", "but", "or", "yet", "so", "and so", "and then", "either...or", "neither...nor", "not only...but also".

She ran to school
and threw her bag down in the corridor.  

Independent clause
Independent clause

* "she" has been omitted due to ellipsis. See ellipsis.

Karl went on camp.
but the others stayed at home.

Independent clause
Independent clause

You could ring her at the office.
or try her on her cell phone.

Independent clause
Independent clause

Complex sentences
In complex sentences, there is a clause expressing the main message and another clause (or clauses) which elaborates on the main message in some way. While the main message is independent, the other clause cannot stand on its own: it is dependent.

They smiled
as if they meant it.  

Independent clause
Dependent clause  

The plant died
because it wasn't watered.  

Independent clause
Dependent clause  

If the container has a hole in it
the raspberries will leak all over the bench.

Dependent clause
Independent clause

Compound-complex sentences
Compound-complex sentences contain a combination of independent and dependent clauses.

The girls walked out of the shop
and laughed loudly*
as they headed for the car.

Independent clause
Independent clause
Dependent clause

* "they" has been omitted due to ellipsis. See ellipsis.

An elderly man was admitted to hospital
after suffering a heart attack
and falling down his stairs.

Independent clause
Dependent clause
Dependent clause

Compound, complex, and compound-complex sentences as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Students need to know the meaning of the conjunctions and other linking words if they are to appreciate the relationship between the ideas.

Research indicates that many students don’t fully recognize these relationships and therefore have difficulty in following the meaning of a text. It has been found for example the conditional "if" is understood by only fifty percent of children at age six; that the meaning of "unless" is often not understood well until after the age of nine; and it is generally not until around twelve years of age that many children comprehend the concessive use of "although". (Deriwianka, 2005, p.95)

(b) Adding information to a sentence
Embedded clauses, e.g., adjectival clauses. Adjectival clauses provide extra information about the noun.            

noun
Rodents
extra information
introduced by settlers

killed the native birds.

The following is an alternative to using an adjectival clause:
Settlers introduced rodents.
These rodents killed the native birds.                          

noun
Native trees
extra information
that block people’s views
or get in the way of property development

are sometimes poisoned.

The following is an alternative to using an adjectival clause:

  • Native trees can block people’s views.
  • Native trees can get in the way of property development.
  • Trees like these are sometimes poisoned.

These types of constructions are often found in simplified text, but students need to gradually develop understanding of how to comprehend and produce sentences with embedded clauses.

Adjectival clauses as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Students need to be able to identify what the adjectival clause is referring to. To do this, they need know:

  • How pronouns function (adjectival clauses often begin with a pronoun, e.g., "who", "whose", "which", "that", "where"). These identify what is being referred to.
  • Which pronoun is omitted, e.g., "the coat [that] I'm wearing".
  • That words other than pronouns are sometimes used, e.g., "introduced by settlers".
  • How commas function e.g., in "New Zealanders who don't eat fruit and vegetables will have poor health", the adjectival clause defines which particular New Zealanders are being referred to. But in "New  Zealanders, who don't eat fruit and vegetables, will have poor health", the adjectival clause is referring to New Zealanders in general.

Modals
Modal verbs convey a range of judgments about the likelihood of events. There are nine modal verbs: "can", "could", "may", "might", "will", "would", "shall", "should", and "must".

  • She might be there.
  • You could get there by lunchtime if you hurry.
  • You'll hit the roof.
  •  That must have hurt.

To show a high degree of certainty about the likelihood of events, modal verbs of high modality are used. If we feel tentative about something, we use low modality.

High modality Medium modality Low modality
must
ought to
has to
will
should
can
need to
may
might
could
would

(Derewianka, 2005, p.66)

As well as modal verbs, modality can be expressed through choices of nouns, adjectives, and adverbs.

  • Modal nouns: "possibility", "probability", "obligation", "necessity", "requirement"
  • Modal adjectives: "possible", "probable", "obligatory", "necessary", "required", "determined"
  • Modal adverbs: "possibly", "probably", "perhaps", "maybe", "sometimes’, "always’ "definitely", "never", "certainly"

Modals as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Students will need to understand modals in order to use the appropriate degree of probability or obligation, especially when using text types that involve making judgments, e.g., narrative texts, and persuading people, e.g., arguments, advertisements.

 

2. The focus of information in a sentence

(a) Grammatical theme (within a sentence)
The sentence theme, which is the information before the main verb in a sentence, usually tells us what the sentence is about, or what the writer wants the reader to focus on. It is a signpost saying to the reader, "This is what I want you to pay attention to". Themes can be:

  • The subject, e.g., Mat usually does Rob's car on a Saturday afternoon... 
  • Conjunctions, e.g., Until the two years is up.... 
  • Elements of an interpersonal nature, e.g., Amazingly, the back wall of the garage is OK.... 
  • Experiential elements such as prepositions, e.g., By the end, he’s sweating and puffing.... 
  • Adverbial elements of circumstance, e.g., By 1840, many missionaries had settled....

Theme (within a sentence) as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Students need to understand that a variety of linguistic elements can serve as sentence themes. They also need to understand that a writer will move an element to the front of the clause to give it emphasis, e.g., "The waters around New Zealand are abundant in delicious seafood such as squid. Although not easy to prepare, squid are..."

(b) New and "given" information
There are usually two types of information in a sentence. One part tells us something new. The other part tells us something that we are aware of already (either from a previous sentence or from our general knowledge), in other words, its information is given.

  • A: Where did you put your bike?
  • B: I left it / at my friend's house.

The first part of B’s sentence is "given" (by A); the second part is new. "Given" information tells us what a sentence is about; it usually provides the sentence theme which appears first in a sentence. New information, however, provides the point where we expect people to pay extra attention.

New and given information as sources of difficulty in comprehension
Students will need to understand that, although given information is usually in the theme position, new information is sometimes the theme.

  • Electricity makes light by....
  • Candles make light by....
  • The sun makes light by....

When this is the case, as it sometimes is in texts written for young children, tracking the development of the text is made more difficult. Difficulty in comprehension can also occur when texts are written in ways that inadvertently draw the reader's attention away from new information. In the following example, the theme contains new information, vital to the completion of a task, but the word 'After' draws attention away from the new information.

  • After reading the text,
  • complete the chart.

Instead, the instruction should read:

  • Read the text,
  • then complete the chart.

 

3. The way language condenses and generalises text

(a) Nominalisation
Nominalisation changes verbs and other words into nouns. It makes a text more compact and "written", e.g., instead of saying "When your body reaches an abnormally low temperature, you will need to be taken to hospital", we can use nominalisations: "Hypothermia requires hospitalisation." Instead of saying "How farmers protected their livestock from the storm was the topic of the article", we can use nominalisation: "Livestock protection was the topic of the article." Changing a verb into a noun requires adding a suffix to the verb:  

Verb + suffix Noun (these examples are all abstract)
break
explore
supervise
farm
star
refuse
amaze
-age
-(a)tion
-ion
-ing
-dom
-al
-ment
breakage
exploration
supervision
farming
stardom
refusal
amazement
 

Nominalisation as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Because nominalisation packs more information into the theme position, students need to process more ideas per clause. It also often makes concrete processes more abstract, or generalises experience, e.g., "Water and wind erode rock" is nominalised as "Erosion occurs when rocks are ground down by water and wind." Students will need to learn how to unpack this kind of condensed language.

(b) Ellipsis
Ellipsis occurs when part of a sentence is left out because it would otherwise repeat what is said elsewhere. The understanding and use of ellipsis increases with maturity and language use. In, "I'd like to eat that biscuit, but I won’t", the second clause is elliptical, with "eat that biscuit" being omitted. In "Read the first paragraph, then the questions", the second clause is elliptical, with "read" being omitted. People usually find the full form of such sentences unnecessary or irritating, and use ellipsis to achieve a more acceptable economy of statement.

Ellipsis is particularly common in conversation:

  • A: Where are you going?
  • B: To the shops. (i.e., I’m going to the shops)
  • A: Why? (i.e., Why are you going to the shops?)
  • B: To get some bread. (i.e., I’m going to the shops to get some bread)
  • A: Is John going with you? (i.e., Is John going with you to the shops to get some bread?)
    (Crystal, 2003, p.228)

Ellipsis as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Students will need to know that they will be able to find out what has been omitted by referring back to the words in the first clause. However, successfully doing this is dependent on the student having at least an intuitive understanding of English grammar.

 

Part two: Connecting and tracking ideas in text 

1. The words used to link ideas within a sentence

Conjunctions
Conjunctions are the words used to link clauses, in this case, independent and dependent. Conjunctions only operate within a sentence, not between sentences and other longer pieces of text. Conjunctions are placed at the beginning of a clause.

Different types of conjunctions are used to express different types of relationships between ideas.

 
  Conjunctions (not a complete list) Example
Place Where, wherever

Wherever I go, I bump into my relatives.

She left it where she found it.

Time After, before, when, just as, as, while

He realized he had lost it when he arrived home.

Before I decide, I want to talk to you.

Manner The way that, as, by

The way that she spends money, you'd think she'd won lotto.

By working overtime, she managed to finish the project.

Cause

As, because, since

 

I came because he saw me.

Since you obviously aren't interested, let's leave it.

Condition As long as, if, unless

If she wants to come, she'll have to hurry up.

Never sit on an ants' nest unless you're wearing cast-iron pants.

Concession

Although, while, even though

Even though they weren't hungry, they ate a full meal.

While recognizing his skill, I don't think he's right for the job.

(Derewianka, 2005, p.96)

Conjunctions as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Sometimes the conjunction is omitted, and the relationship between the clauses has to be inferred, e.g., Andrew felt tremendous relief. He had got the marks he needed for uni. He slumped down into the chair, and breathed a long, slow sigh. In this case, students will have to infer the relationship i.e., Andrew felt tremendous relief because he had got the marks he needed for uni, so he slumped down into the chair, and breathed a long, slow sigh. Also see "Compound, complex, and compound-complex sentences as a source of difficulty in comprehension".

 


2. The words used to link ideas within and between sentences

(a) Cohesion
Cohesive devices (grammatical and lexical) make links between various items in the text so that the reader is able to track how meaning is being developed.

Referring words (grammatical cohesion)
Referring words set up links by referring to something that has already been mentioned. The most commonly used referring words are pronouns. Others are the definite article (the), pointing words, and words that replace verb and noun groups and even whole clauses

Pronouns
In the following example the pronouns keep referring back to the main characters:              

  • Hansel and Gretel lived in a small thatched cottage at the edge of a large forest. Their father had recently married a greedy and selfish woman...they...them....         

In the following example, the pronoun makes a reference link to Hansel's pockets:

  • Hansel stuffed his pockets with as much bread as they would hold.

The definite article (the)
In the following example, "the" is used to refer back to something which has been introduced previously and which we can now take for granted:

  • A small house at the edge of the forest... the house....

Pointing words ("this", "that", "these", "those")
In the following example "This" refers back to the verb group "Yoghurt can be strained through muslin."

  • Yoghurt can be strained through muslin. This produces thicker yoghurt and a by-product called whey.

In the following example "These" refers back to the noun group "three stigmas".

  • The saffron crocus produces three stigmas. These are used in cooking.

In the following example "These" refers to the noun group "hard outer shells".

  • The nuts are encased in hard outer shells. These are inedible.

Words which replace verb and noun groups and even whole clauses:
In the following example "did" refers to the verb group "to stick a chicken bone through the bars of the cage."

  • Gretel told Hansel to stick a chicken bone through the bars of the cage. And he did.

In the following example "one" replaces the noun group "pieces of bread."

  • I've got two pieces of bread. Do you want one?

In the following example "so" replaces the clause, "She was sick of feeding them."

  • She was sick of feeding them. They heard her say so.
Referring words as a source of difficulty in comprehension
In order to appreciate how ideas are linked between sentences, students will need to:

  • Know which words are being referred to, i.e., a verb group, a noun group, or a whole clause, e.g.,
  • Gretel told Hansel to stick a chicken bone through the bars of the cage. And he did. (verb group)
  • I've got two pieces of bread. Do you want one? (noun group)
  • She was sick of feeding them. They heard her say so. (whole clause)
  • Know that pronouns and definite articles always refer to the noun group, and that pointing words usually do but can also refer to the verb group.
  • Know which word the pointing word refers to when there are two possible plural referents, e.g., "nuts" and "hard outer shells". The nuts are encased in hard outer shells. These are inedible.

Text connectives (grammatical cohesion)
Text connectives provide signposts indicating how the text is developing. Unlike conjunctions, they can be placed at various positions in the sentence. Also unlike conjunctions, which form links within a sentence, they form links between sentences and other longer pieces of text.

(Not a complete list)

Clarifying Showing cause/result Indicating time
in other words
for example
that is
namely
in fact
so
therefore
consequently
due to..., owing to
because of this
then
next
finally
meanwhile
previously
Sequencing ideas Adding information Condition/concession
firstly, first, second, third...
at this point
to conclude
given the above points
to get back to the point
too
in addition
also
again
similarly
in that case
however
despite this
even so
if not
(Derewianka, 2005, p.110–111)

Text connectives as a source of difficulty in comprehension
In order to appreciate how ideas are linked between sentences, students will need to:

  • Know the meaning of the connective.
  • Know that while connectives and conjunctions often form very similar functions, unlike conjunctions, connectives operate across sentences, and can be placed at various positions within the sentence.

Word associations (lexical cohesion)
Like cohesive devices, word associations form links within texts.

Repetition
This is the most simple kind of lexical cohesion. Text participants can easily be tracked because they are referred to using the same words throughout the text. Synonyms; antonyms; hyponyms; hypernyms. Using synonyms, antonyms, hyponyms, and hypernyms forms less direct links in the text than repetition, but adds interest and subtlety to the text. In the sentence "My dad bought a new car",

  • "bought" can be replaced by "purchased" (synonym—similar meaning)
  • "bought" can be replaced by "sold" (antonym—contrasting meaning)
  • "car" can be replaced by "Ford" (hyponym—more specific meaning)
  • "car" can be replaced by "vehicle" (hypernym—more general meaning)

Collocation
Collocation is a term used for words which typically occur together, making a text predictable. In fairy tales the words "Once upon a time", "wicked stepmother", "wicked witch", and "lived happily ever after" collocate. Collocations cannot be predicted from a knowledge of the world. For example, "heavy" collocates with "loss", "wear’, "traffic", ‘burden", "defeat" etc. but not with "war", "win", "stress", or "mistake". All that is required for a sequence of words to be described as a collocation, is for one word to "call up" another, to some extent, in the mind of a native or fluent speaker of English.

Collocational norms are intentionally broken when the writer wants to be inventive. Poets in particular, are likely to break collocational conventions, sometimes to a point where coherent meaning is difficult to find. The following gives examples where a poet has deliberately broken collocational norms. The examples range in difficulty from reasonably easy to very difficult to understand.

Examples where collocational norms are broken (Metaphors taken from Dylan Thomas’s After the Funeral).

Some degree of expectancy Unusual Well beyond expectations. We are forced to search for meanings
Humble hands
Mourning house

Skyward statue

Crooked year
threadbare whisper
damp word
round pain
(Crystal, 2003, p.163)
 

Word associations as a source of difficulty in comprehension:

  • Students will need to work particularly hard to comprehend when words do not seem to collocate.
  • Word associations add interest and subtlety to the text, but can make tracking meaning difficult.

(b) Theme (between sentences)
The clause theme tells us what the clause is about. It is a signpost saying to the reader: "this is what I want you to pay attention to." Because the theme appears first in a clause, and so first in a sentence, it focuses our attention on how the topic is being developed. The theme helps to make the text coherent and enables the reader to predict how the text is unfolding. The following is an example of student writing from School Journal Part 4, Number 3, 2005 p.30, To Spray or Not to Spray? The highlighted sections show the themes.

Our school was in the spray zone, and the first spray day was pretty scary. Our principal told us that if they saw the spray plane, there would be a special bell. When we heard the bell, we were to go inside, or if we heard a plane, we were to tell everybody and get inside quickly and shut the windows and doors. The big kids were to help the little kids.

We didn't hear the big plane until it flew over. Then the bell went. The plane flew over heaps. It was really low and really loud. The smell was pretty yuck - sort of like cat wee. The plane sprayed at playtime. Some of us were playing in the bush away from the classrooms, so it was pretty scary. We had to go running inside.

That was the only spray day like that. The four others so far have been different. After that, MAF stopped spraying between 8 and 9 a.m. so that we could get to school. They also stopped spraying between 12.15 and 1.00 p.m. and between 3 and 4 p.m., which was good.

Theme (between sentences) as a source of difficulty in comprehension
Students need to understand how to track the development of the text through identifying themes. To do this, they will need to know that:

  • Paragraphs begin with a general thematic point, with later sentences elaborating from it.
  • New information at the end of a sentence is often picked up as the theme for the next. (The above example is typical of children's writing in that the sentences tend to jump from one to the next without building on previous information).
  • The relationship between sentences is made explicit through the use of connectives e.g., "Then" and "After that".
  • Various items in the text are linked by referring words. (The above example is again typical of children's writing in that it is not entirely clear what “That” and “After that” in paragraph three are referring to.)
References
Crystal, D. (2003). The Cambridge encyclopedia of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Derewianka, B. (2005). A grammar companion. Newtown, NSW: PETA.